978-0077825362 Chapter 7 Part 2

subject Type Homework Help
subject Pages 9
subject Words 2128
subject Authors Eugene Zechmeister, Jeanne Zechmeister, John Shaughnessy

Unlock document.

This document is partially blurred.
Unlock all pages and 1 million more documents.
Get Access
page-pf1
16
mean of the two unilateral conditions (i.e., the mean of left and right) was statistically
significant. What would you claim about the bilateral superiority effect?
page-pf2
17
LEARNING BY DOING RESEARCH
A simple experiment involving repeated measures that would also be familiar to students is a Ataste test.@
These procedures could be used as a class demonstration or students could conduct this simple
experiment with friends as participants. Presented here are procedures that could be followed for
comparing two conditions, but it would be easy to expand the number of conditions depending on students’
research question. An advantage of the taste-test experiment is that students can appreciate the
complications associated with differential transfer in the repeated measures design.
Step 1: Students choose their research question. For example, students might ask, ACan people
distinguish between Coke and Diet Coke?@ (a third condition might test Coke Zero), or ACan people tell the
difference between Coke and Pepsi?@ (a third condition might test a generic product). Instead of Atell the
difference@ questions, students may choose a question relating to preference (e.g., ADo people prefer
Coke or Pepsi?@). Students may identify other products of interest for comparison.
Step 2: Define the dependent variable. To illustrate computational procedures using the repeated measures
design, students could use a rating scale for each taste sample. For example, for a research question
regarding whether people can distinguish between products, participants could rate the likelihood that each
product is Coke on a 1-10 scale. They would make the rating for each taste sample. If the research question
concerns preference, the dependent variable question could ask participants to use a rating scale anchored
by Ado not like at all@ to Alike very much@ for each taste sample.
Step 3: Decide what type of repeated measures design to use. Students should decide whether each
participant samples each product once (incomplete design) or more than once (complete design). Once this
decision is made, students should decide the method for balancing practice effects in their design.
Step 4: Design the procedure for recording participants= responses based on the dependent variable and
repeated measures design selected. Other decisions, such as how much of each product should be tasted,
how to keep participants (and experimenters) blind to condition, random assignment to orders of conditions,
and extraneous variables to hold constant, should be addressed.
Step 5: Tabulate the results, making sure to Aunwind@ the order of conditions and product condition (i.e.,
Aproduct A@ will appear in the 1st ordinal position and the 2nd ordinal position, etc.). Use descriptive
statistics to describe the findings, and inferential statistics as appropriate to the classroom situation.
Step 6: Address issues of differential transfer: Is it possible that sampling AProduct A@ first influences the
taste of AProduct B,@ and vice versa? A simple way to test for the possibility of differential transfer is to
compare the overall results for the repeated measures design with the results for just the first ordinal
position. Are ratings of the products different in the first ordinal position, in which no prior tasting could
influence participants= ratings, compared to the overall results?
Step 7: Students may be asked to write a brief report to describe their findings. In their report they should
describe their research question; the research design, including how the independent variable was
manipulated and counterbalancing procedures; the dependent variable; a summary of the results, including
descriptive and inferential statistics (as appropriate); a discussion of their test for differential transfer; and
their conclusions.
page-pf3
18
INSTRUCTOR=S LECTURE/DISCUSSION AIDS
The following pages outline content from Chapter 7 and may be used to facilitate lecture or discussion.
1. Repeated Measures Designs: This page describes the repeated measures design and the reasons
researchers use this design.
2. Practice Effects: This page illustrates the problem of practice effects using an example.
3. Balancing Practice Effects: This page introduces complete and incomplete repeated measures designs
for counterbalancing practice effects.
4-5. Complete Repeated Measures Design: These two pages describe the complete repeated measures
design, block randomization and ABBA counterbalancing.
6. Incomplete Repeated Measures Design: The incomplete design is differentiated from the complete
design on this page.
7-8. Counterbalancing in the Incomplete Design: Methods for counterbalancing, all possible orders and
selected orders (Latin square, random starting order with rotation) are described on these two pages.
9. Data Analysis for Repeated Measures Designs: This page identifies the additional step required when
participants complete the experimental conditions in the complete design.
10. The Problem of Differential Transfer: This page describes differential transfer.
11. Comparison of Two Designs: This page compares repeated measures designs and random groups
designs on two dimensions: independent variable and what is controlled through balancing.
page-pf4
19
Repeated Measures Designs
! Each individual participates in each condition of the experiment.
Completes DV measure with each condition
Hence, Arepeated measures@
! Also called Awithin-subject design@
Entire experiment is completed Awithin@ each subject
! Advantages
No need to balance individual differences across conditions of
experiment
Fewer participants needed
Convenient and efficient
More sensitive design
! A sensitive experiment can detect the effect of an IV
even if the effect is small
! Error variation is reduced
! Variability due to individual differences is eliminated.
page-pf5
20
Practice Effects
! Main disadvantage of repeated measures designs
People change as they are tested repeatedly.
! Performance may improve over time.
! People may become tired or bored as number of Atrials@ increases.
! Practice effects become a potential confounding variable if not controlled.
! Example
Researcher tests 2 study methods, A and B, for participants’
comprehension of text passages
! Condition A: highlight text while studying, then take test.
! Condition B: Read text and create sample test questions and answers,
then take test.
Suppose researchers tests all participants in condition A first, then B.
Results show test scores are higher with Method A than Method B.
Problem: Confounding of study method IV with order of presentation
! Can’t determine effect of IV
! Practice effects (boredom, fatigue) may explain poorer performance
for Method B.
page-pf6
21
Balancing Practice Effects
! Balance practice effects across conditions
! Counterbalance the order of conditions
Half of the participants do Condition A first, then Condition B.
The remaining participants do Condition B, then A.
! Distribute practice effects equally across conditions
Practice effects aren’t eliminated.
Balance, or average, practice effects across the conditions of
experiment.
! Two types of repeated measures designs
Complete repeated measures design
Incomplete repeated measures design
! Complete and incomplete designs differ in how practice effects are
counterbalanced.
page-pf7
22
Complete Repeated Measures Design
! Balance practice effects within each participant.
Each participant experiences each condition several times.
Each participant forms a Acomplete@ experiment.
Use different orders each time.
Best when each condition is brief
e.g., simple judgments about stimuli
! Two methods for counterbalancing order of conditions
Block randomization
ABBA counterbalancing
! Block randomization
page-pf8
23
Complete Repeated Measures Design, continued
! ABBA counterbalancing
Present conditions only a few times to each participant
Use one random sequence of conditions (e.g., DABC)
Then the opposite of the sequence (CBAD).
Repeat with new random sequence and its opposite, etc.
When anticipation effects can occur:
Participants form expectations about which condition will appear next
page-pf9
24
in sequence.
Responses may be influenced by expectations, not independent
variable.
If anticipation effects are likely (e.g., conditions are predictable), use
block randomization.
page-pfa
25
Incomplete Repeated Measures Design
! Each participant experiences each condition once.
Not many times, as in the complete design.
! Balance practice effects across participants (not within)
! General rule for balancing practice effects:
Each condition must appear in each ordinal position (1st, 2nd, 3rd, etc.)
equally often.
! Two methods for balancing practice effects
All possible orders
Selected orders
page-pfb
26
Counterbalancing in the Incomplete Design
! All possible orders
Use with 4 or fewer independent variable conditions
2 conditions (A, B) 2 possible orders: AB, BA
Randomly assign half of participants to do condition A then B,
other half do condition B then A
3 conditions (A, B, C) 6 possible orders:
ABC, ACB, BAC, BCA, CAB, CBA
Randomly assign participants to one of the 6 orders
4 conditions (A, B, C, D) 24 possible orders
Need at least 1 participant randomly assigned to each order.
! Selected orders
Select particular orders of conditions to balance practice effects
Two methods: Latin Square and Random Starting Order with Rotation
Each IV condition appears in each ordinal position exactly once.
page-pfc
27
Selected Orders for Counterbalancing
in the Incomplete Design
! Latin square
Ordinal Position
1st 2nd 3rd 4th
A B D C
B C A D
C D B A
D A C B
Each condition appears in each ordinal position to balance practice
effects.
Another advantage: Each condition precedes and follows every other
condition once (AB and BA, BC and CB, etc.).
This helps to control for possible order effects.
! Random starting order with rotation
Generate random order of conditions (e.g, ABCD)
Rotation: Move each condition one position
Ordinal Position
1st 2nd 3rd 4th
A B C D
B C D A
C D A B
D A B C
page-pfd
28
Each condition appears in each ordinal position.
Possible order effects are not balanced.
Data Analysis for Repeated Measures Designs
! Complete repeated measures design requires one additional step.
Compute a summary score (e.g., mean) for each participant for each
independent variable condition.
This score represents each participant=s average performance in
each condition.
page-pfe
29
The Problem of Differential Transfer
! Do not use repeated measures designs when differential transfer is
possible.
Effects of one condition persist and affect participants’ experience of
subsequent conditions.
Use independent groups design instead.
Assess whether differential transfer is a problem by comparing results
for repeated measures design and random groups design.
Compare performance in 1st ordinal position to overall results.
1st 2nd 3rd 4th
A B D C
B C A D
C D B A
D A C B
1st ordinal position represents a random groups design.
Differential transfer may be a problem if performance in 1st ordinal
position differs from results for entire repeated measures design.
page-pff
30
Comparison of Two Designs
! Differences between repeated measures design and independent
(random) groups design
! Compare how independent variable is manipulated
Repeated measures: each participant experiences every condition of
the IV
Independent (random) groups design: each participant experiences
only one condition of the IV
! Compare what is balanced across conditions in order to rule out
alternative explanations for findings (confoundings)
Repeated measures: practice effects
Independent (random) groups: individual differences

Trusted by Thousands of
Students

Here are what students say about us.

Copyright ©2022 All rights reserved. | CoursePaper is not sponsored or endorsed by any college or university.